Niko Tinbergen - Simple English Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Niko Tinbergen | |
---|---|
Born | |
Died | 21 December 1988 | (aged 81)
Nationality | Dutch |
Alma mater | Leiden University |
Known for | Hawk/goose effect Four questions |
Awards | Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine (1973) |
Scientific career | |
Fields | Zoologist, ethologist |
Institutions | Oxford University |
Doctoral students | Richard Dawkins Aubrey Manning Desmond Morris |
Nikolaas 'Niko' Tinbergen (15 April 1907 – 21 December 1988) was a Dutch ethologist and ornithologist who shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine with Karl von Frisch and Konrad Lorenz. Their discoveries were about individual and social behaviour patterns, and how they are triggered.
In the 1960s Niko collaborated on a series of wildlife movies, including The Riddle of the Rook (1972) and Signals for Survival (1969), which won the Italia prize in that year and the American blue ribbon in 1971.
Aspects of his life
[change | change source]Niko, who had two eminent brothers, Luuk and Jan, was a prisoner of war during World War II. His experience as a prisoner of the Nazis led to some friction with longtime intellectual collaborator Konrad Lorenz, and it was several years before the two reconciled.
After the war, Tinbergen moved to England, where he taught at the University of Oxford. He remained in England for the rest of his life. Several of his Oxford graduate students went on to become prominent biologists; these include Richard Dawkins, Marian Dawkins, Desmond Morris, and Iain Douglas Hamilton. He married Elisabeth Rutten and they had five children.
Supernormal stimuli
[change | change source]The body of Tinbergen's research focused on what he termed supernormal stimuli. This was the concept that an artificial object could be a stronger trigger for an instinct than the natural object for which the instinct originally evolved.[1]
He constructed plaster eggs to see which a bird preferred to sit on, finding that they would select those that were larger, had more defined markings, or more saturated colour—and a dayglo-bright one with black polkadots would be selected over the bird's own pale, dappled eggs.[2]
Tinbergen found that territorial male stickleback fish would attack a wooden fish model more vigorously than a real male if its underside was redder. He constructed cardboard dummy butterflies with more defined markings that male butterflies would try to mate with in preference to real females. The superstimulus, by its exaggerations, clearly showed what features were triggering the response.
Four Questions
[change | change source]He is well known for originating the four questions he believed should be asked of any animal behaviour,[3] which were:
Proximate mechanisms:
- 1. Causation (Mechanism): what are the stimuli that elicit the response, and how has it been modified by recent learning?
- 2. Development (Ontogeny): how does the behaviour change with age, and what early experiences are necessary for the behaviour to be shown?
Ultimate mechanisms:
- 3. Evolution (Phylogeny): how does the behaviour compare with similar behaviour in related species, and how might it have arisen?
- 4. Function (Adaptation): how does the behaviour impact on the animal's chances of survival and reproduction?
Autism
[change | change source]Niko gave his Nobel Lecture on autism in children, which he and his wife had been researching for some time.[4]
References
[change | change source]- Hans Kruuk 2003. Niko's nature: the life of Niko Tinbergen and his science of animal behaviour ISBN 0-19-851558-8
- ↑ Tinbergen, Niko 1951. The study of instinct. Oxford, Clarendon Press.
- ↑ Tinbergen, Niko 1953. The Herring Gull's world. London, Collins.
- ↑ Tinbergen, Niko 1963: On aims and methods in ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20: 410-433 [1] Archived 2011-06-09 at the Wayback Machine
- ↑ Tinbergen on autism